top of page

How People Learn: Insights from Neuroscience

  • Writer: Enseigne Education
    Enseigne Education
  • Apr 1
  • 6 min read

Understanding how people learn has been a major focus of neuroscience research, leading to significant insights into brain function and cognitive development. Empirical evidence from neuroscience has demonstrated that learning involves changes in neural structures, synaptic connections, and cognitive processes.


Neuroplasticity: The Brain's Ability to Change

One of the most significant discoveries in neuroscience is neuroplasticity, which refers to the brain’s ability to reorganize itself by forming new neural connections in response to learning and experience. Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) have demonstrated that leaHow People Learn: Insights from Neurosciencerning strengthens specific neural pathways (Draganski et al., 2004). For example, research on London taxi drivers found that their hippocampus, a region associated with spatial memory, was more developed due to their navigation experience (Maguire et al., 2000). This suggests that repeated learning and practice lead to structural brain changes.


Synaptic Plasticity and Long-Term Potentiation (LTP)

Synaptic plasticity is the process by which the strength of synapses (connections between neurons) changes over time in response to experience. Long-term potentiation (LTP), a key mechanism in synaptic plasticity, has been extensively studied in the hippocampus and is considered essential for memory formation (Bliss & Lømo, 1973). When neurons repeatedly activate together, the synaptic connection between them becomes stronger, enhancing the efficiency of neural circuits related to learning (Malenka & Bear, 2004).


Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Executive Function and Learning

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is critical for higher-order cognitive functions such as reasoning, decision-making, and problem-solving. Neuroscientific research shows that the PFC is heavily involved in working memory and goal-directed learning (Miller & Cohen, 2001). Studies on children and adolescents suggest that as the PFC matures, individuals become better at self-regulation, attention, and abstract thinking, which improves learning outcomes (Casey et al., 2005).


Dopamine and the Reward System in Learning

The brain’s reward system, particularly the release of dopamine in the mesolimbic pathway, plays a crucial role in motivation and reinforcement learning. Studies have shown that dopamine release enhances memory retention and encourages repeated engagement in learning activities (Schultz, 1997). Positive reinforcement, such as praise or rewards, has been found to improve learning efficiency by increasing dopamine levels and strengthening neural circuits related to motivation (Wise, 2004).


The Importance of Sleep in Memory Consolidation

Sleep is essential for learning and memory consolidation. Research using polysomnography and fMRI has revealed that during sleep, particularly in slow-wave and REM sleep, newly acquired information is transferred from short-term to long-term memory storage in the hippocampus and neocortex (Diekelmann & Born, 2010). Sleep deprivation has been found to impair cognitive function and reduce the brain’s ability to form and retrieve memories (Walker & Stickgold, 2006).


The Impact of Stress on Learning

Stress and emotional states significantly influence learning. The release of cortisol during stress can have both positive and negative effects on memory. Short-term stress can enhance focus and memory retention, whereas chronic stress has been linked to impairments in hippocampal function and reduced learning capacity (Lupien et al., 2009). Mindfulness and stress-reduction techniques have been found to improve cognitive performance and learning outcomes (Zeidan et al., 2010).


Conclusion

Empirical evidence from neuroscience provides a deeper understanding of how learning occurs in the brain. Neuroplasticity, synaptic plasticity, executive function, dopamine-mediated motivation, sleep, and stress all play crucial roles in shaping learning and memory. By leveraging these insights, educators, students, and professionals can optimize learning strategies to enhance knowledge retention and skill acquisition.




人類如何學習:來自神經科學的啟示


瞭解人類如何學習一直是神經科學研究的重要課題,該領域的研究揭示了學習如何影響大腦結構、突觸連結和認知過程。基於實證研究,科學家已經確定了學習涉及神經可塑性、突觸可塑性、多巴胺獎勵系統、睡眠對記憶的影響等多個關鍵因素。


神經可塑性:大腦的可變性

神經可塑性(Neuroplasticity)指的是大腦透過學習和經驗調整自身結構和功能的能力。功能性磁振造影(fMRI)和腦電圖(EEG)研究表明,學習會強化特定的神經通路(Draganski et al., 2004)。例如,倫敦計程車司機的海馬迴(Hippocampus)因長期記憶與導航相關資訊而顯著增長(Maguire et al., 2000)。這證明重複練習與學習會促使大腦結構發生變化。


突觸可塑性與長期增強作用(LTP)

突觸可塑性(Synaptic Plasticity)指的是突觸連接的強度會根據經驗而改變。長期增強作用(Long-Term Potentiation, LTP)是突觸可塑性的關鍵機制,特別是在海馬迴內部對於記憶形成至關重要(Bliss & Lømo, 1973)。當神經元反覆活化時,突觸之間的連結變得更強,使相關的神經迴路更高效(Malenka & Bear, 2004)。


前額葉皮質在執行功能與學習中的角色

前額葉皮質(Prefrontal Cortex, PFC)負責高階認知功能,如推理、決策和問題解決。研究表明,PFC 在工作記憶(Working Memory)和目標導向學習中扮演關鍵角色(Miller & Cohen, 2001)。針對兒童與青少年的研究發現,隨著 PFC 發育成熟,個體在自我調控、注意力和抽象思維方面的能力提升,從而改善學習效果(Casey et al., 2005)。


多巴胺與獎勵系統在學習中的作用

大腦的獎勵系統(Reward System),特別是中腦邊緣系統(Mesolimbic Pathway)中多巴胺(Dopamine)的釋放,對於動機與強化學習(Reinforcement Learning)至關重要。研究表明,多巴胺的釋放可以提升記憶保留,並促使個體持續參與學習活動(Schultz, 1997)。正向增強(如讚美或獎勵)會增加多巴胺水平,加強與學習動機相關的神經迴路(Wise, 2004)。


睡眠對記憶鞏固的重要性

睡眠對於學習與記憶鞏固至關重要。多項研究(包括多導睡眠監測和 fMRI 研究)顯示,在睡眠期間,特別是慢波睡眠(Slow-Wave Sleep)和快速動眼期睡眠(REM Sleep)階段,新學習的信息會從短期記憶轉移到長期記憶,並儲存在海馬迴與大腦皮質(Diekelmann & Born, 2010)。睡眠不足則會損害認知功能,降低大腦的學習與記憶能力(Walker & Stickgold, 2006)。


壓力對學習的影響

壓力與情緒狀態會顯著影響學習。皮質醇(Cortisol)在壓力狀態下的釋放可能對記憶產生雙重影響:短期壓力可能增強專注力與記憶,而長期壓力則會損害海馬迴功能並降低學習能力(Lupien et al., 2009)。研究發現,正念冥想(Mindfulness Meditation)和壓力管理技巧有助於提高認知表現並增強學習能力(Zeidan et al., 2010)。


結論

神經科學的實證研究為我們提供了更深入的學習機制理解。神經可塑性、突觸可塑性、執行功能、多巴胺獎勵系統、睡眠和壓力都在學習與記憶過程中扮演關鍵角色。這些研究成果不僅有助於個人優化學習策略,也為教育工作者設計更有效的教學方法提供了依據。



References


Bliss, T. V., & Lomo, T. (1973). Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. The Journal of physiology, 232(2), 331–356. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273 


Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development?. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(3), 104–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011


Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 11(2), 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762 


Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311–312. https://doi.org/10.1038/427311a


Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 10(6), 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639


Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(8), 4398–4403. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.070039597


Malenka, R. C., & Bear, M. F. (2004). LTP and LTD: an embarrassment of riches. Neuron, 44(1), 5–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.012 


Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual review of neuroscience, 24, 167–202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167


Schultz W. (1997). Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Current opinion in neurobiology, 7(2), 191–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80007-4


Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annual review of psychology, 57, 139–166. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070307


Wise R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 5(6), 483–494. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1406


Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and cognition, 19(2), 597–605. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014 

 
 
 

コメント


bottom of page